Objectives. We investigated the relationship among father’s occupational group, daily smoking, and smoking determinants in a cohort of New Zealand adolescents.

Methods. The longitudinal Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study provided information on adolescents’ self-reported smoking behavior and potential predictors of smoking, such as social and material factors, personality characteristics, educational achievement, and individual attitudes and beliefs regarding smoking. Longitudinal logistic generalized estimating equation analyses were used.

Results. Adolescents whose fathers were classified in the lowest-status occupational group were twice as likely as those whose fathers occupied the highest-status occupational group to be daily smokers. This high risk of daily smoking among the adolescents from the lowest occupational group was largely predicted by their lower intelligence scores and by the higher prevalence of smoking among fathers and friends .

Conclusions. To prevent socioeconomic differences in smoking, school-based interventions should seek to prevent smoking uptake among adolescents, particularly those of lower socioeconomic status. Programs need to provide positive, nonsmoking role models consonant with the culture and norms of lower-socioeconomic-status groups. Adolescents need to acquire resistance skills and protective behaviors against social pressure and influences.

Attempts to describe and explain socioeconomic differences in unhealthy behavior have mainly focused on adults. But lifestyle patterns observed among adults are largely developed during adolescence and are perpetuated into adulthood. Little is known about the emergence of socioeconomic differences in unhealthy lifestyles during adolescence and even less about the determinants of this process. Such information would greatly facilitate the design of effective interventions to prevent the development of socioeconomic differences in behavior at an early stage.

The objective of our study was to examine patterns and predictors of socioeconomic differences in adolescent smoking behavior. The longitudinal Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study followed a birth cohort of approximately 1000 individuals during their entire adolescence and thus provides a unique opportunity to describe and explain the relationship between father’s occupational group and adolescents’ daily smoking. This article is among the first to report on the contribution by a variety of determinants of adolescent smoking to the association between father’s occupational group and daily smoking among adolescents.

A review of the literature shows that adolescents of lower socioeconomic status (SES) smoke more often than do their peers of higher SES,1–13 though some studies fail to find such a relationship.14–16 This hypothetical relationship between parental SES and adolescent smoking might originate from a higher prevalence of determinants of adolescent smoking among lower-SES groups. To date, not many predictors of adolescent smoking behavior have been investigated regarding their association with SES, and only 2 other studies have analyzed the contribution of such predictors to socioeconomic differences in smoking behavior.4,12

The adolescent smoking literature emphasizes the effect of modeling behavior of parents and peers. Children who have smoking parents (or who live with people who smoke)4,5,8,10,12,14,15,17–27 or who have friends who smoke1,2,6,8,12,18–24,28 are more inclined to start smoking during adolescence, though some studies have not found such a relationship.6,7,28,29 Other social factors that are reported to predict smoking during adolescence are perceived approval or pressure to smoke,2,7,18,22,27,30–32 poor family support or control,6,12,14,22,24,26,33 poor social bonding,2 and high involvement in social activities.30

Seltzer and Oechsli9 addressed the predictive potential of personality traits with regard to adolescent smoking and reported that children with type-A personality traits, extraversion, and psychoticism are more likely to begin smoking during adolescence than are children without these characteristics. (A person that scores high on psychoticism will exhibit some qualities commonly found among psychotic persons. Examples of psychotic tendencies include recklessness, disregard for common sense, and inappropriate emotional expression.) External locus of control,11,18,34 low self-esteem,1,2,11,22,23,26,35 and deviant or risky behavior2,18,22,23,25,26,35 are also related to adolescent smoking. Individual positive attitudes and beliefs related to smoking2,6,17,18,27,28,30,36 predict adolescent smoking, though McNeill et al7 found no such a relationship.

Educational achievement also plays a role in adolescent smoking. Poorer school achievement,4,19,23 negative attitudes toward or poor adjustment in school,17,22 low academic expectations,2,17,18,24,26 and average or below-average school performance1,17,20,22,25 all predict smoking during adolescence. (Murray and colleagues,30 however, found no relationship between attitude toward school or truancy and smoking during adolescence.)

In general, material factors are considered important determinants of socioeconomic differences in health or health-related behavior.37–39 Smoking behavior during adolescence is predicted by the availability of money.2,7

Although the relationship between predictors of adolescents’ smoking behavior and SES has not been the subject of many studies, some of the above-mentioned predictors are reported to be more prevalent in lower SES groups. Adolescents of lower SES are more likely than those of higher SES to have smoking parents, friends, peers, and siblings10,12,23; they also experience more social pressure to smoke and positive norms involving smoking,10 and they more often report an external locus of control,11–12 lower self-esteem,12 and poorer academic achievement.12 We hypothesized that these determinants contribute to socioeconomic differences in adolescent smoking.

Population

Data were obtained from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study, which is a longitudinal investigation of the health, development, and behavior of a cohort of children from birth until adulthood.40 In brief, the sample consists of a cohort born in Dunedin’s only obstetric hospital between April 1, 1972, and March 31, 1973. The perinatal histories were documented soon after birth, but study participants were first enrolled in the longitudinal study at 3 years of age. Ninety-one percent of eligible births (i.e., of people still resident in the province of Otago) participated in the first assessment, providing a baseline sample of 1037 participants. Study participants were assessed every 2 years up to age 15 and then at ages 18, 21, and 26. Most participants were assessed within 2 months of their birthdays. Transportation to the research unit was provided for participants living in New Zealand (but outside Dunedin). During the assessment that was carried out when the study members were 21 years old, an interviewer traveled to those living overseas (almost all in Australia). This procedure resulted in very high follow-up rates: between 90% and 97% of the study participants included in the baseline sample, with a onetime low follow-up rate of 82% at age 13 years.40 Before the interviews, informed consent was obtained from a parent (for interviews of participants younger than 18 years) and from the participant (for interviews of participants aged 15 and older).

The sample was representative of the population of New Zealand’s South Island in terms of parent’s SES, children’s educational achievement, and ethnicity (primarily European).40

Measures

The questions on smoking behavior were first included in the study when study participants reached 9 years of age. When participants were 9, 11, and 13 years old, the interviews regarding smoking were conducted by the same trained interviewer. The interviews were conducted in private at the research unit as part of the series of assessments of health, development, and behavior during 1 day. A small portion of the sample was unable to attend the research unit for assessment at ages 9, 11, and 13 years; these participants were assessed at home or at school and were not asked about smoking. At ages 15, 18, and 21 years, smoking questions were included in the home, school, or workplace interviews. Most of these interviews were conducted by the same interviewer.

Regular daily smoking is often used as an indicator of the development of habitual smoking. In this sample, a comparison of self-reported smoking status with saliva cotinine concentrations showed high sensitivity (96%) and specificity (82%) of self-reported data.41

Father’s occupational group was assessed at the beginning of adolescence (age 9 years) and was categorized according to the Elley-Irving classification.42 This classification was designed for use in New Zealand but is internationally comparable to other occupational classifications, because it is based on the International Standard Classification of Occupations. Average income and education levels (based on the 1981 New Zealand census for males) were used to rate fathers’ occupations.43 We selected father’s occupation to indicate adolescent’s SES, with the selection criteria set out by Liberatos and colleagues, to maximize comparability of our study with other studies.44 When studying children or adolescents, social class is typically measured according to parental occupation, usually the father or the head of the household.4,9,10,16,45

Adolescents’ health is equally related to occupation-based SES measures and to nonoccupational SES measures, e.g., father’s education, household income, neighborhood deprivation, housing tenure, and car availability. However, on balance, the occupational measures seem to be the better discriminators.45 When information on father’s occupation was missing at a particular age, information collected at later measurements was used. Because of low numbers among the 2 lowest occupational categories, the semi-skilled and unskilled groups, we combined these groups.

The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study measured information on several potential predictors of smoking behavior among adolescents, including social and material factors, personality characteristics, personal attitudes and beliefs regarding smoking factors, and educational achievement.

Social factors included self-reported smoking behavior of mothers and fathers as well as parents’ reports regarding family relationships.46,47 The study participants reported on smoking of members of their household, smoking of close friends, their attachment to family and friends,48 their relationship with their parents, and their participation in organized groups, clubs, or other activities. Material factors were reported by the parents and covered receipt of pocket money by children, the number of children in the family, and occurrence of father’s unemployment in the 2 years preceding the interview.

Personality characteristics were measured with Rosenberg’s questions regarding self-esteem,49 Rutter’s neuroticism questionnaire,50 the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control scales,51,52 and the Quay and Peterson behavioral problems checklist.53

Individual attitudes and beliefs included attitudes toward smoking friends or adults, as well as toward smoking in general, beliefs that smoking is as bad for one’s health as people say and that smoking will affect health when one is older, and the number of reasons to smoke that could be enumerated by study participants.

Educational achievement was indicated by performance at school and the Wechsler intelligence score.54

To identify risk groups, we divided all continuous-scale variables into tertiles (3 equally sized groups) or into 2 groups, comparing the top or bottom quartile with the rest of the study population.

Analyses

Analyses were conducted in 4 stages. At the first stage, we examined the relationship between father’s occupational group and daily smoking. We calculated the prevalence of daily smoking by SES for each of the assessments when participants were 9, 11, 13, 15, 18, and 21 years old. Because none of the participants smoked daily at ages 9 or 11, we omitted these age groups from further longitudinal analyses. We then fitted logistic regression models, adjusted for gender, for each measurement wave separately, with the highest-status occupational group as a reference category. Next, we fitted a longitudinal logistic generalized estimating equation (GEE) model accounting for the dependence between repeated measurements within the same individual, with the GENMOD procedure of SAS 8.0.55 We calculated occupational differences in daily smoking for the period between 13 years and 21 years by fitting a GEE model including gender, time, and father’s occupational group. This longitudinal GEE analysis used information on 947 study participants, of whom 50.8% were boys and 49.2% were girls. The magnitude of socioeconomic differences in daily smoking did not differ between boys and girls; that is, there was no significant interaction between father’s occupational group and the child’s gender.

At the second stage, we examined which variables longitudinally predicted daily smoking for the period between 13 years and 21 years by fitting GEE models including gender, time, and 1 potential determinant successively. Variables were considered to be predictors of daily smoking when the GEE analyses showed a significant likelihood ratio χ2 test (P < .05) and significantly increased odds ratios (P < .05).

At the third stage, for those predictors that showed significantly increased odds of daily smoking, we examined the distribution of the predictor among occupational groups.

Finally, at stage 4, we added significant predictors of daily smoking occurring more often among adolescents of lower SES to the first GEE model (including gender, time, and occupation) in an attempt to explain the relationship between father’s occupational group and adolescents’ daily smoking. The contribution of this predictor to the explanation of differences in smoking by SES was expressed by the percentage reduction in odds ratios of the different occupational groups owing to the inclusion of the predictor (all significantly increased odds ratios of occupation should decrease in value).

Stage 1

In this New Zealand cohort of adolescents born in 1972, daily smoking was observed to begin at 13 years (Table 1). From that time, the smoking prevalence increased drastically with each measurement wave, reaching adult levels of smoking prevalence at 18 years, when about one third of the respondents reported being daily smokers. Differences in daily smoking by father’s occupational group emerged at 15 years, at which age smoking prevalence clearly decreased with an increase in father’s occupational group (Table 1). Relative differences in daily smoking were statistically significant from age 15 onward, although only for the 2 lowest-status occupational groups (Table 1).

Longitudinal GEE analyses, which take into account all repeated measurements from the entire adolescent period beginning with 13 years, showed that adolescents in the 2 lowest-status occupational groups were twice as likely as those in the highest-status occupational group to be daily smokers, whereas adolescents in the second-lowest-status occupational group had odds of daily smoking more than 1½ times higher (Table 1). Longitudinal GEE analyses further revealed that occupational differences in adolescent daily smoking were stable during the entire period studied (P value = .4352 for occupation × time interaction).

Stage 2

Table 2 shows the potential predictors measured at baseline that predicted daily smoking during adolescence (13 years through 21 years). We found that several social factors—having a smoking (or formerly smoking) father or smoking friends, living with smokers, not belonging to any group or organization, having poor family relationships, and having low attachment to parents—significantly predicted daily smoking during adolescence (Table 2). The only material factor that significantly increased the odds of being a daily smoker was receiving pocket money (Table 2). Adolescents who reported behavioral problems showed significantly increased odds of daily smoking (Table 2). Furthermore, adolescents who had positive attitudes toward smoking friends or adults, who did not believe in the detrimental effects of smoking, who reported a higher number of reasons to smoke were significantly more likely to smoke daily (Table 2). Compared with adolescents who strongly agreed with the statements about the harmfulness of smoking, those who just agreed had higher odds of smoking daily (Table 2). Low and medium intelligence scores and average or below-average school performance significantly predicted daily smoking (Table 2).

Stage 3

Table 3 shows the relationship between father’s occupational group and statistically significant risk categories of predictors of daily smoking. A significant inverse relationship with father’s occupational group was observed for having a smoking father or friend, poor family relationships, behavioral problems, average or below-average school performance, or low intelligence scores. Also, number of reasons to smoke was significantly related to father’s occupational group; unexpectedly, adolescents of higher SES reported higher numbers of reasons to smoke (Table 3).

We also observed that some risk factors were clearly more prevalent in the lowest occupational group: living with smokers, not belonging to an organization, having positive attitudes toward smoking friends, and not believing in the adverse health effects of smoking.

Stage 4

We tested the explanatory potential of all predictors of daily smoking occurring significantly more often among groups of adolescents whose fathers reported lower-status occupational group (we excluded attachment to parents, receipt of pocket money, and reasons to smoke from stage 4 of the analyses). We found that in this New Zealand population, intelligence scores and the smoking behavior of father and friends explained the relationship between father’s occupational group and daily smoking during adolescence: together these predictors reduced the significantly increased odds ratios of skilled, semiskilled, and unskilled occupational groups to nonsignificant levels. The unequal distribution of intelligence scores across occupational groups contributed most to explaining the association between father’s occupational group and adolescent daily smoking (Table 4).

We observed a clear relationship between father’s occupational group and daily smoking during adolescence among this New Zealand cohort. Longitudinal analyses revealed that occupational differences in adolescent smoking were present and stable from the onset of daily smoking. The lower IQ scores of adolescents whose fathers reported a lower-status occupational group contributed most to the relationship between father’s occupational group and daily smoking. Also, the smoking behaviors of father and friends explained part of the differences in daily smoking among adolescents according to father’s occupational group. None of the material factors, personality characteristics, or individual beliefs and attitudes contributed to the relationship between father’s occupational group and adolescent smoking.

Some limitations of the data must be acknowledged. First, father’s occupational group might vary over time during the long study period. However, the significantly high correlation among the 4 measurements of father’s occupational group in this study—between 9 years and 15 years (r > 0.70, P = .0)—indicates that occupational group was largely stable. Second, to exclude all possible concerns about causality between predictors and smoking behavior, we chose to include factors measured at the baseline of our longitudinal analyses, that is, before or at age 13. Because we studied a relatively long time frame, it is possible that the effect of factors that affect smoking in the short term or that are likely to change is underestimated. For example, behavioral attitudes or material barriers are very likely to affect current behavior, but this effect might dissipate over time, resulting in weaker associations using longer time frames. Other factors, such as intelligence, are more likely to influence behavior throughout the adolescent period.

The most important predictor of occupational differences in daily smoking during adolescence was lower intelligence scores among children of lower SES. The rare studies that have investigated explanations for socioeconomic differences in adolescent smoking and substance use also have identified academic competence or achievement as one of the explanatory pathways.4,12 Less-intelligent adolescents might use smoking as a substitute for the satisfaction of academic success. They might also be less receptive to messages about the negative health consequences of antismoking programs.

Intelligence likely results not only from heredity but also from environmental influences.38,56–59 Reviews of long-term effects of early childhood education and day care found persistent positive effects of these interventions on academic achievement60,61 and future SES,60 as well as on IQ.58–60 Greater access to such interventions for lower-SES groups might help prevent, or at least delay, adolescent smoking.

The role of intelligence may reflect the mechanism by which socioeconomic differences in smoking among adults are established. Adolescents with lower intelligence levels are less likely to pursue higher education. In our New Zealand cohort, lower intelligence scores at age 13 were related to having a lower-status occupation at 21 years (P = .0162). Furthermore, adolescent smoking is known to lead to poor educational achievement and therefore low SES.62,63 To break the persistent cycle of socioeconomic differences in smoking, intervention programs should focus on preventing smoking uptake among adolescents, particularly those of lower SES. This prevention might be achieved by developing school-based interventions in lower-SES neighborhoods.

The contribution of the smoking behaviors of fathers and friends to the differences in adolescent smoking highlights the importance of modeling behavior during adolescence.64 The few studies that have attempted to explain socioeconomic differences in smoking among adolescents indicate an effect of parents’ and friends’ smoking behavior.4,12 Although adolescents of lower SES had more exposure to smoking role models, we found that they were not more vulnerable to the influence of these role models (nonsignificant interaction of occupation× smoking father/friends). Interventions to prevent smoking among adolescents should provide positive, nonsmoking role models who fit the culture and norms of lower-status occupational groups.31 Furthermore, adolescents can resist social pressure and influences in favor of smoking if they are taught resistance skills or protective behaviors.2 Relevant programs need to consider and effectively involve adolescents’ social environment, that is, their parents and social communities.1 This involvement accords with US guidelines for school health programs to prevent tobacco use and addiction.65 Such interventions should be targeted toward adolescents belonging to lower-SES groups, because they are disproportionately exposed to potent predictors of smoking and are thus at greater risk of becoming daily smokers.

Table
TABLE 1— Association Between Daily Smoking and Father’s Occupational Group, by Age in Years
TABLE 1— Association Between Daily Smoking and Father’s Occupational Group, by Age in Years
 Percentage (OR)a 
 91113151821ORb
Father’s occupational group (distribution in population)
    Higher professional, administrative (14%). . .. . .1.1 (1.00)8.1 (1.00)22.3 (1.00)29.6 (1.00)1.00
    Lower professional, technical (14%). . .. . .1.1 (0.98)8.7 (1.06)25.4 (1.17)27.6 (0.90)0.97
    Clerical, highly skilled (26%). . .. . .1.5 (1.40)14.1 (1.86)28.9 (1.41)31.5 (1.10)1.24
    Skilled (29%). . .. . .0.5 (0.42)14.5 (1.94)33.5 (1.75)37.5 (1.43)1.57
    Semiskilled, unskilled (17%). . .. . .1.8 (1.65)19.5 (2.86)38.4 (2.21)45.4 (1.99)2.12
Overall prevalence001.113.530.334.7. . .
Pc. . .. . ..8804.0208.0217.0087.0029
No. assessed955925850976993992. . .
No. interviewed on smoking779794734964937903. . .

Note. GEE = generalized estimating equation; OR = odds ratio.

aOdds ratio of logistic regression analysis adjusted for gender for each measurement wave separately.

bOdds ratio of longitudinal logistic GEE analysis, which included youths aged 13–21 years, adjusted for gender.

cP value of likelihood ratio χ2 test of father’s occupational group.

Table
TABLE 2— Association Between Potential Predictors of Smoking Behavior and Daily Smoking During Adolescence
TABLE 2— Association Between Potential Predictors of Smoking Behavior and Daily Smoking During Adolescence
 ORaPb
Social factors
Smoking behavior of mother .1332
    Nonsmoker1.00 
    Ex-smoker1.15 
    Smoker1.35 
Smoking behavior of father .0091
    Nonsmoker1.00 
    Ex-smoker1.54 
    Smoker1.55 
Smoking behavior in the house .0023
    No1.00 
    Yes6.05 
Smoking behavior of close friends ≤.0001
    No one smokes1.00 
    1 or more smoke(s)3.26 
Participates in organized groups, clubs, or activities .0019
    Yes1.00 
    No1.74 
Family relationships ≤.0001
    Better relationships1.00 
    Quartile with poorest relationship1.93 
Attachment to parents .0107
    High1.00 
    Medium1.35 
    Low1.74 
Relationship with parents .0773
    Okay1.00 
    Not (always) okay2.50 
Attachment to friends .2274
    Low1.00 
    Medium0.91 
    High0.72 
Individual attitudes and beliefs
Attitude toward smoking friends .0070
    Neutral1.00 
    Prefer nonsmokers0.65 
    Prefer smokers6.54 
Attitude toward smoking adults .0354
    Not okay to smoke in moderation1.00 
    Okay to smoke in moderation1.43 
General attitude toward smoking .4526
    Lower scores1.00 
    Most positive quartile1.13 
Believes smoking is as bad for your health as people say .0
    Strongly agree1.00 
    Agree1.97 
    Disagree or strongly disagree5.03 
Believes smoking will affect health when you are older .0000
    Strongly agree1.00 
    Agree1.79 
    Disagree or strongly disagree12.53 
Number of reasons to smoke .0002
    Low1.00 
    Medium1.24 
    High12.34 
Educational achievement factors
Performance at school .0000
    Above average1.00 
    Average2.29 
    Below average4.19 
Intelligence ≤.0001
    High1.00 
    Medium1.40 
    Low3.21 
Personality characteristics
Self-esteem .4883
    High1.00 
    Medium0.93 
    Low1.25 
Health locus of control .0687
    Internal1.00 
    Neutral1.17 
    External1.49 
Neuroticism .3040
    Lower scores1.00 
    Highest quartile (most neurotic)0.85 
Behavioral problems ≤.0001
    Low1.00 
    Medium1.54 
    High2.46 
Material factors
Receives pocket money .0419
    No1.00 
    Yes1.31 
No. of children in family .5804
    1 or 21.00 
    30.94 
    4 or more1.04 
Father registered as unemployed .0936
    No1.00 
    Yes1.88 

Note. OR = odds ratio.

aOdds ratio of generalized estimating equation analyses, which included youths aged 13–21 years, adjusted for gender.

bP value of likelihood ratio χ2 test of generalized estimating equation analysis. Values rounded to 4 decimal places.

Table
TABLE 3— Prevalence of Risk Categories of Predictors of Daily Smoking During Adolescence, by Father’s Occupational Group
TABLE 3— Prevalence of Risk Categories of Predictors of Daily Smoking During Adolescence, by Father’s Occupational Group
 Father’s Occupational Group, % 
 LowestaLower bIntermediatecHigher dHighest ePf
Social factors
Smoking behavior of father     .000
    Ex-smoker14.520.520.228.317.2 
    Smoker56.545.441.233.625.9 
Smoking in the house     .129
    Yes11.96.06.14.37.7 
Smoking behavior of close friends     .002
    1 or more smoke49.536.035.133.022.2 
Participates in groups, clubs, or activities     .315
    No27.918.724.218.921.1 
Family relationships     .001
    Quintile with poorest relationships35.023.315.722.916.7 
Attachment to parents     .077
    Low35.539.732.334.423.6 
Material factors
Receives pocket money     .913
    Yes55.656.559.659.059.7 
Personality characteristics
Behavioral problems     .000
    Medium37.328.838.737.626.4 
    High44.138.125.331.223.6 
Individual attitudes and beliefs
Attitude toward smoking friends     .548
    Prefer smokers1.81.40.00.00.0 
Attitude toward smoking adults     .110
    Okay to smoke in moderation81.680.975.879.668.8 
Believes smoking is bad for health     .448
    Agree36.037.933.038.533.3 
    Disagree or strongly disagree9.94.24.63.36.7 
Believes smoking affects health when older     .090
    Agree33.338.328.942.926.7 
    Disagree or strongly disagree4.51.41.51.12.2 
Number of reasons to smoke     .001
    High29.728.535.638.354.4 
Educational achievement factors
School performance     .000
    Average78.277.963.562.351.1 
    Below average9.16.14.73.32.3 
Intelligence     .000
    Low50.842.129.822.910.9 

aHigher professional, administrative.

bLower professional, technical.

cClerical, highly skilled.

dSkilled.

eSemiskilled, unskilled.

f P value of χ2 test. Values rounded to 3 decimal places.

Table
TABLE 4— Differences in Daily Smoking During Adolescence, by Father’s Occupational Group
TABLE 4— Differences in Daily Smoking During Adolescence, by Father’s Occupational Group
 ORa (% Reduction in OR From Basic Model)b
 Basic Modela,cBasic + IntelligenceBasic + Smoking FatherBasic + Smoking FriendsBasic + All 3 Predictors
Father’s occupational group (reference group: higher professional, administrative)
    Lower professional, technical0.970.950.861.010.88
    Clerical, highly skilled1.241.051.081.000.71
    Skilled1.571.28 (51)1.49 (14)1.38 (33)0.93 (100)
    Semiskilled, unskilled2.121.88 (21)1.93 (17)2.07 (4)1.28 (75)
P value for occupationd.0029.0692.0090.0281.2752

Note. OR = odds ratio.

aOdds ratio of daily smoking during adolescence obtained by longitudinal logistic GEE analysis.

bPercentage reduction in odds ratio of daily smoking owing to inclusion of predictor (OR basic model − OR [basic + predictor])/(OR basic model − 1).

cBasic model for longitudinal logistic generalized estimating equation (GEE) analyses included youths aged 13–21 years, adjusted for gender.

dP value of likelihood ratio χ2 test of father’s occupational group in GEE analysis.

The Van Walree Foundation and the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research financially supported Mariël Droomers at the Alcohol and Public Health Research Unit, University of Auckland, New Zealand.

Droomers wishes to thank Karen Witten and Philippa Howden-Chapman for providing her the opportunity to work in New Zealand and the staff of the Alcohol and Public Health Research Unit for their moral support. Furthermore, the authors would like to thank Richie Poulton and Barry Milne for their indispensable help in accessing the Dunedin data, and Megan Pledger, Elisabeth Robinson, and Gerard Borsboom for their advice in statistical matters.

Human Participant Protection As part of the invitation to attend the research unit, full written explanations of all the procedures were provided, and the written consent of the parents and study participants was obtained.

References

1. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Preventing tobacco use among young people. A report of the Surgeon General. MMWR Recomm Rep. 1994; 43(RR-4):1–10. Google Scholar
2. Conrad KM, Flay BR, Hill D. Why children start smoking cigarettes: predictors of onset. Br J Addict. 1992; 87:1711–1724. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
3. Escobedo LG, Anda RF, Smith PF, Remintron PL, Mast EE. Sociodemographic characteristics of cigarette smoking initiation in the United States. JAMA. 1990; 264:1550–1555. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
4. Green G, Macintyre S, West P, Ecob R. Like parent, like child? Associations between drinking and smoking behaviour of parents and their children. Br J Addict. 1991;86:745–758. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
5. Greenlund KJ, Liu K, Kiefe CI, Yunis C, Dyer AR, Burke GL. Impact of father’s education and parental smoking status on smoking behavior in young adults. Am J Epidemiol. 1995;142:1029–1033. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
6. Mittelmark MB, Murray DM, Luepker RV, Pechacek TF, Pirie PL, Pallonen UE. Predicting experimentation with cigarettes: the Childhood antecedents of Smoking Study (CASS). Am J Public Health. 1987; 77:206–208. LinkGoogle Scholar
7. McNeill AD, Jarvis MJ, Stapleton JA, et al. Prospective study of factors predicting uptake of smoking in adolescence. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1988; 43:72–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
8. Pedersen W, Lavik NJ. Role modelling and cigarette smoking; vulnerable working class girls? A longitudinal study. Scand J Soc Med. 1991;19:110–115. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
9. Seltzer CC, Oechsli FW. Psychosocial characteristics of adolescent smokers before they started smoking: evidence of self-selection. A prospective study. J Chronic Dis. 1985;38:17–26. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
10. De Vries H. Socioeconomic differences in smoking: Dutch adolescents’ beliefs and behavior. Soc Sci Med. 1995;41:419–424. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
11. Wills TA. Self-esteem and perceived control in adolescent substance use: comparative tests in concurrent and prospective analyses. Psychol Addict Behav. 1994;8:223–234. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
12. Wills TA, McNamara G, Vaccaro D. Parental education related to adolescent stress-coping and substance use: development of a mediational model. Health Psychol. 1995;14:464–478. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
13. Lee RE, Cubbin C. Neighborhood context and youth cardiovascular health behaviors. Am J Public Health. 2002;92:428–436. LinkGoogle Scholar
14. Glendinning A, Shucksmith J, Hendry L. Family life and smoking in adolescence. Soc Sci Med. 1997;44:93–101. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
15. Gliksman MD, Dwyer T, Wlodarczyk J, Pierce JP. Cigarette smoking in Australian schoolchildren. Med J Aust. 1989;150:81–84. MedlineGoogle Scholar
16. Tuinstra J, Groothoff JW, van den Heuvel WJA, Post D. Socioeconomic differences in health risk behavior in adolescence: do they exist? Soc Sci Med. 1998; 47:67–74. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
17. Aaro LE, Wold B, Kannas L, Rimpelä M. Health behaviour in schoolchildren, a WHO cross-national survey. Health Prom. 1986;1:17–33. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
18. Chassin L, Presson CC, Sherman SJ, Corty E, Olshavsky RW. Predicting the onset of cigarette smoking in adolescents: a longitudinal study. J Applied Soc Psychol. 1984;14:224–243. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
19. Glendinning A, Shucksmith J, Hendry L. Social class and adolescent smoking behaviour. Soc Sci Med. 1994;38:1449–1460. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
20. Pierce JP, Choi WS, Gilpin EA, Farkas AJ, Berry CC. Tobacco industry promotion of cigarettes and adolescent smoking. JAMA. 1998;279:511–515. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
21. West P, Sweeting H, Ecob R. Family and friends’ influences on the uptake of regular smoking from mid-adolescence to early adulthood. Addiction. 1999;94:1397–1412. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
22. Jackson C. Initial and experimental stages of tobacco and alcohol use during late childhood: relation to peer, parent, and personal risk factors. Addict Behav. 1997;22:685–698. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
23. Chatrou M. Psychological Studies: Determinants of Smoking and Smoking Prevention in Dutch Adolescents. Leiden, The Netherlands: DSWO Press; 1992. Google Scholar
24. Chassin L, Presson CC, Sherman SJ, Edwards DA. Parent educational attainment and adolescent cigarette smoking. J Subst Abuse. 1992;4:219–234. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
25. Jackson C, Henriksen L, Dickinson D, Messer L, Robertson SB. A longitudinal study predicting patterns of cigarette smoking in late childhood. Health Educ Behav. 1998;25:436–447. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
26. Jackson C, Henriksen L, Dickinson D, Levine DW. The early use of alcohol and tobacco: its relation to children’s competence and parents’ behavior. Am J Public Health. 1997;87:359–364. LinkGoogle Scholar
27. Eiser JR, Morgan M, Cammage P, Gray E. Adolescent smoking: attitudes, norms and parental influence. Br J Soc Psychol. 1989;28:193–202. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
28. Hirschman RS, Leventhal H, Glynn K. The development of smoking behavior: conceptualisation and supportive cross-sectional survey data. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1984;14:184–206. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
29. White HR, Johnson V, Buyske S. Parental modeling and parenting behavior effects on offspring alcohol and cigarette use: a growth curve analysis. J Subst Abuse. 2000;12:287–310. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
30. Murray M, Swan AV, Bewley BR, Johnson MR. The development of smoking during adolescence: the MRC/Derbyshire smoking study. Int J Epidemiol. 1983; 12:185–192. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
31. Eckert P. Beyond the statistics of adolescent smoking. Am J Public Health. 1983;73:439–441. LinkGoogle Scholar
32. De Vries H, Backbier E, Kok G, Dijkstra M. The impact of social influences in the context of attitude, self-efficacy, intention, and previous behaviour as predictor of smoking onset. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1995;25:237–257. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
33. Foxcroft DR, Lowe G. Adolescent drinking, smoking and other substance use involvement: links with perceived family life. J Adolesc. 1995;18:159–177. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
34. Clarke JH, Macpherson BV, Holmes DR. Cigarette smoking and external locus of control among young adolescents. J Health Soc Behav. 1982;23:253–259. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
35. Sussman S, Dent CW, Stacy AW, et al. Peer-group association and adolescent tobacco use. J Abnorm Psychol. 1990;99:349–352. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
36. Andrews JA, Duncan SC. The effect of attitude on the development of adolescent cigarette use. J Subst Abuse. 1998;10:1–7. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
37. Davey Smith G, Blane D, Bartley M. Explanations for socio-economic differentials in mortality. Evidence form Britain and elsewhere. Eur J Public Health. 1994; 4:131–144. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
38. Townsend P, Davidson N. The Black report. In: Townsend P, Davidson N, Whitehead M, eds. Inequalities in Health. London, England: Penguin Group; 1988:32–213. Google Scholar
39. Schrijvers CTM, Stronks K, van de Mheen HD, Mackenbach JP. Explaining educational differences in mortality: the role of behavioral and material factors. Am J Public Health. 1999;89:535–540. LinkGoogle Scholar
40. Silva PA, Stanton WR, eds. From Child to Adult. The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study. Auckland, New Zealand: Oxford University Press; 1996. Google Scholar
41. Stanton WR, McClelland M, Elwood C, Ferry D, Silva PA. Prevalence, reliability, and bias of adolescents’ reports of smoking and quitting. Addiction. 1996;91:1705–1714. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
42. Elley WB, Irving JC. Revised socio-economic index for New Zealand. N Z J Educ Res. 1976;11:25–56. Google Scholar
43. Elley WB, Irving JC. The Elley-Irving socioeconomic index: 1981 census revision. N Z J Educ Res. 1985;20:115–128. Google Scholar
44. Liberatos P, Link BG, Kelsey JL. The measurement of social class in epidemiology. Epidemiol Rev. 1988;10:87–121. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
45. MacIntyre S, West P. Lack of social class variation in health in adolescence: an artefact of an occupational measure of social class? Soc Sci Med. 1991;32:395–402. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
46. Holahan CJ, Moos RH. The quality of social support: measure of family and work relationships. Br J Clin Psychol. 1983;22:157–162. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
47. Moos R, Moos B. Family Environment Scale Test Manual. Palo Alto, Calif: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1986. Google Scholar
48. Nada Raja S, McGee R, Stanton WR. Perceived attachments to parents and peers and psychological well-being in adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 1992;21:471–485. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
49. Rosenberg M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1965. Google Scholar
50. Rutter M, Tizard J, Whitmore K. Education, Health and Behaviour. London, England: Longman; 1970. Google Scholar
51. Stanton WR, Nada Raja S, Langley J. Stability in the structure of health locus of control among adolescents. Br J Clin Psychol. 1995;34:279–87. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
52. Wallston KA, Wallston BS, DeVellis R. Development of the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control (MHLC) Scales. Health Educ Monogr. 1978;6:160–170. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
53. Quay HC, Peterson DR. Manual for the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist. Coral Gables, Fla: University of Miami; 1987. Google Scholar
54. Wechsler D. Manual of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised. New York, NY: Psychological Corp; 1974. Google Scholar
55. Liang KY, Zeger SL. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika. 1986;73:13–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
56. Scarr S, McCartney K. How people make their own environments: a theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Dev. 1983;54:424–435. MedlineGoogle Scholar
57. Caspi A, Harkeness AR, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. Intellectual perfomance: continuity and change. In: Silva PA, Stanton WR, eds. From Child to Adult: The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study. Auckland, New Zealand: Oxford University Press; 1996:59–74. Google Scholar
58. Martin SL, Ramey CT, Ramey S. The prevention of intellectual impairment in children of impoverished families: findings of a randomized trial of educational day care. Am J Public Health. 1990;80:844–847. LinkGoogle Scholar
59. Campbell FA, Ramey CT. Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: a follow-up study of children from low-income families. Child Dev. 1994;65:684–698. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
60. Zoritch B, Roberts I, Oackley A. The health and welfare effects of day care: a systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Soc Sci Med. 1998;47:317–327. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
61. Barnett WS. Long-term cognitive and academic effects of early childhood education on children in poverty. Prev Med. 1998;27:204–207. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
62. Koivusilta LK, Rimpelä AH, Rimpelä MK. Health-related lifestyle in adolescence: origin of social class differences in health? Health Educ Research. 1999;14:339–355. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
63. Ellickson PL, Tucker JS, Klein DJ. High-risk behaviors associated with early smoking: results from a 5-year follow-up. J Adolesc Health. 2001;28:465–473. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
64. Bandura A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. New York, NY: Prentice Hall; 1986. Google Scholar
65. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Guidelines for school health programs to prevent tobacco use and addiction. MMWR Recomm Rep. 1994; 43(RR-2):1–18. Google Scholar

Related

No related items

TOOLS

SHARE

ARTICLE CITATION

Mariël Droomers, PhD, Carola T.M. Schrijvers, PhD, Sally Casswell, PhD, and Johan P. Mackenbach, PhD, MDAt the time of the study, Mariël Droomers, Carola T. M. Schrijvers, and Johan P. Mackenbach were with the Department of Public Health, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, The Netherlands. Sally Casswell is with the Centre for Social and Health Outcomes Research Evaluation, Massey University, Auckland, New Zealand. “Father’s Occupational Group and Daily Smoking During Adolescence: Patterns and Predictors”, American Journal of Public Health 95, no. 4 (April 1, 2005): pp. 681-688.

https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2002.002774

PMID: 15798130