Objectives. We examined whether perceived chronic discrimination was related to excess body fat accumulation in a random, multiethnic, population-based sample of US adults.

Methods. We used multivariate multinomial logistic regression and logistic regression analyses to examine the relationship between interpersonal experiences of perceived chronic discrimination and body mass index and high-risk waist circumference.

Results. Consistent with other studies, our analyses showed that perceived unfair treatment was associated with increased abdominal obesity. Compared with Irish, Jewish, Polish, and Italian Whites who did not experience perceived chronic discrimination, Irish, Jewish, Polish, and Italian Whites who perceived chronic discrimination were 2 to 6 times more likely to have a high-risk waist circumference. No significant relationship between perceived discrimination and the obesity measures was found among the other Whites, Blacks, or Hispanics.

Conclusions. These findings are not completely unsupported. White ethnic groups including Polish, Italians, Jews, and Irish have historically been discriminated against in the United States, and other recent research suggests that they experience higher levels of perceived discrimination than do other Whites and that these experiences adversely affect their health.

It is estimated that 2 of every 3 adults in the United States are overweight or obese.1,2 Obesity is a major risk factor for chronic health conditions, such as type 2 diabetes, coronary heart disease, hypertension, stroke, some forms of cancer, and osteoarthritis.3 Although it is widely accepted that high-fat diets and physical inactivity are preventable risk factors,4 obesity continues to increase.1,2,5

There is a growing interest in the relationship between psychosocial risk factors and excess body fat accumulation.616 In particular, some evidence suggests that psychosocial stressors may play a role in disease progression in general and in excess body fat in particular.7,8,17 The key factors underlying physiological reactions to psychosocial stress have not been completely elucidated, but McEwen and Seeman17 and others7,18,19 posit that the continued adaptation of the physiological system to external challenges alters the normal physiological stress reaction pathways and that these changes are related to adverse health outcomes.8,17,18,20 For example, in examining the association between psychosocial stress and excess body fat accumulation, Björntorp and others have suggested that psychosocial stress is linked to obesity, especially in the abdominal area.7,8

Perceived discrimination, as a psychosocial stressor, is now receiving increased attention in the empirical health literature.2124 Such studies suggest perceived discrimination is inversely related to poor mental and physical health outcomes and risk factors, including hypertension,24,25 depressive symptoms,2628 smoking,2931 alcohol drinking,32,33 low birthweight,34,35 and cardiovascular outcomes.3638

Internalized racism, the acceptance of negative stereotypes by the stigmatized group,39 has also been recognized as a race-related psychosocial risk factor.40 Recent studies have also suggested that race-related beliefs and experiences including perceived discrimination might be potentially related to excess body fat accumulation. Three of these studies9,13,41 showed that internalized racism was associated with an increased likelihood of overweight or abdominal obesity among Black Caribbean women in Dominica41 and Barbados13 and adolescent girls in Barbados.9 These researchers posit that individuals with relatively high levels of internalized racism have adopted a defeatist mindset, which is believed to be related to the physiological pathway associated with excess body fat accumulation. However, Vines et al.16 found that perceived racism was associated with lower waist-to-hip ratios among Black women in the United States. Although the assessment of race-related risk factors varied across these studies, the findings suggest that the salience of race-related beliefs and experiences may be related to excess body fat accumulation.

Collectively, the results of these studies are limited. First, because they examined the relationship between race-related beliefs and experiences and excess body fat only among women, we do not know if this relationship is generalizable to men.13,16,41 Second, these studies only examined this relationship among Blacks, even though perceived unfair treatment because of race/ethnicity has been shown to be adversely related to the health of multiple racial/ethnic population groups in the United States4249 and internationally.27,5055 Third, none of the studies have examined the relationship between excess body fat accumulation and perceived nonracial/nonethnic experiences of interpersonal discrimination. Some evidence suggests that the generic perception of unfair treatment or bias is adversely related to health, regardless of whether it is attributed to race, ethnicity, or some other reason.45,55,56 Fourth, none of these studies included other measures of stress. We do not know if the association between race-related risk factors and obesity is independent of other traditional indicators of stress.

Using a multiethnic, population-based sample of adults, we examined the association of perceived discrimination and obesity independent of other known risk factors for obesity, including stressful major life events. Additionally, because reports of perceived racial/ethnic discrimination and non-racial/ethnic discrimination vary by racial/ethnic groups24,45,46,57 and because Whites tend to have less excess body fat than do Blacks and Hispanics,1,3 we examined the relationships between perceived discrimination and excess body fat accumulation among Hispanics, non-Hispanic Whites, and non-Hispanic Blacks.

Participants

We analyzed data collected between May 2001 and March 2003 from the Chicago Community Adult Health Study (CCAHS), a stratified, multistage probability sample of 3105 adults 18 years and older living in Chicago, Illinois.58 The CCAHS obtained self-reported measures of individual and household psychological risk factors and resources, environment, and social contexts. The sample included 802 Hispanics, 1240 non-Hispanic Blacks (Blacks), 983 non-Hispanic Whites (Whites), and 80 individuals of other races/ethnicities. We excluded the “other” racial/ethnic group because of small sample size (n = 80). One individual per household was interviewed face-to-face for a response rate of 71.82%. The weighted sample matches the distribution of the 2000 Census population estimates for the city of Chicago in age, race or ethnicity, and gender.59

Anthropometric Measures

There are 2 dependent variables for this study, body mass index (BMI; weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared, an indicator for general body fatness) and waist circumference. During the interview process, following standardized procedures from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, trained interviewers measured each respondent's height without shoes, weight with a calibrated scale, and waist circumference with a tape measure.60 The 3 BMI categories used for this study were underweight and normal ( < 25.0 kg/m2), overweight (25.0–29.9 kg/m2), and obese ( ≥ 30.0 kg/m2).3 Waist circumference, an aggregate measurement of total and abdominal fat accumulation, is considered to be superior for assessing abdominal adiposity because of its high correlation with visceral adiposity and high predictive value for subclinical metabolic disease.6167 We used a gender-specific dichotomous measure indicating high-risk waist circumference: greater than 88 cm for women and greater than 102 cm for men.4

Perceived Discrimination Variables

We assessed perceived discrimination with a 5-item version of the everyday discrimination scale, a measure of perceived chronic interpersonal discrimination that assesses the occurrence and frequency with which individuals encounter routine and relatively minor experiences of unfair treatment.57 Specifically, respondents reported their perception of how often (1) they were treated with less courtesy or respect than others, (2) they received poorer service than others, (3) they believed others acted as if they were not smart, (4) others acted as if they were afraid of them, or (5) they felt threatened or harassed. The response scale ranged from 1 to 5, with 1 indicating the highest frequency of at least once per week to 5 indicating never. The 5 items in this study have an internal consistency of 0.75 with the full sample. The everyday discrimination scale, with a range of 0 to 20, was created by reverse coding (response option 5 = 0; 4 = 1; 3 = 2; 2 = 3; 1 = 4) and summing across the 5 items.

After answering all of the questions, respondents were asked to provide a main reason why they thought these experiences had happened to them. Respondents who indicated ancestry, national origin, or race as the main reason for the unfair treatment were classified as having experienced racial/ethnic discrimination; any other reason, such as some other physical attribute, gender, or age, was coded as nonracial/nonethnic discrimination. Because the frequencies of both discrimination variables were skewed, and after some exploratory analyses with categorical variables based on quartile and quantile cutoff points, we created dichotomized variables that contrast any versus no reports of perceived discrimination.68

Covariates

The other covariates used in this analysis were: age in years, education (dummy coded as < 12 years, 12–15 years, and ≥ 16 years), income (dummy coded as < $10 000, $10 000–$29 999, $30 000–$49 999, > $49 999, and missing), drinking status (dummy coded as nondrinker, past drinker, and current drinker), smoking status (dummy coded as nonsmoker, past smoker, and current smoker), self-reported physical activity (dummy coded as never, light, moderate, regular, and vigorous regular), and stressful major life events (a summary measure of the count of major life events, ranging from 0 to 11).

Analytic Plan

All analyses in this study were weighted to take into account the different rates of selection, as well as household size, differential coverage, and nonresponse across each of the neighborhood clusters. To ensure that the standard errors were correctly calculated, because of the complex sampling design, we used the complex survey feature of Stata software version 9.2 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX), which uses first-order Taylor series linearization. We first calculated the weighted distribution of all of the variables of interest for the entire sample and then for each of the 3 racial/ethnic groups. Multinomial logistic regression and logistic regression analyses were conducted to examine the relationship between interpersonal experiences of perceived discrimination and the 2 outcome measures, BMI and waist circumference, controlled for individual-level characteristics. A series of analyses was performed with the covariates added in an iterative manner; however, only the final models are presented here.

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the entire sample and separately for Hispanics, Blacks, and Whites. Forty percent of all respondents had a high-risk waist circumference and more than two thirds of the sample were either overweight or obese. A larger proportion of Blacks and Hispanics had a high-risk waist circumference and was either overweight or obese compared with Whites.

Table

TABLE 1 Sample characteristics: Chicago Community Adult Health Study Sample, Chicago, IL, 2001–2003

TABLE 1 Sample characteristics: Chicago Community Adult Health Study Sample, Chicago, IL, 2001–2003

Full Samplea (N = 3025)Hispanics (n = 811)Non-Hispanic Whites (n = 1206)Non-Hispanic Blacks (n = 1008)
With at-risk waist circumference,b %40.041.3***29.050.3***
BMI, %
    Underweight or normal weight ( < 25.0 kg/m2)30.519.2***42.025.9***
    Overweight (25.0–29.9 kg/m2)34.139.7*33.829.9
    Obese ( ≥ 30.0 kg/m2)35.441.1***24.244.2***
Everyday discrimination, %
    Racial/ethnic25.630.5***7.842.9***
    Any nonracial/nonethnic41.027.3***55.634.8***
    Some other aspect of physical appearancec11.714.411.710.3
    Genderc11.23.9***17.99.1***
    Agec11.110.915.57.1***
    None33.342.236.622.3***
Major life events, mean no. (SE)2.8 (0.1)2.5 (0.1)2.6 (0.1)3.1 (0.1)***
Education, y, %
    < 1224.244.7***11.023.4***
    12–1549.245.243.759.0***
    ≥ 1626.710.1***45.317.6***
Income, $, %
    < 10 00011.510.16.618.6***
    10 000–29 99925.532.8***17.129.6***
    30 000–49 99918.421.017.517.3
    ≥ 50 00026.118.0***37.818.7***
    Missing18.518.121.015.8*
Smoking status, %
    Nonsmoker54.466.1***49.251.1
    Past smoker20.516.0***25.817.7***
    Current smoker25.518.3**25.431.3**
Drinking status, %
    Nondrinker18.024.7***11.620.3***
    Past drinker19.720.8*15.024.2***
    Current drinker62.454.5***73.055.2***
Physical activity index, %
    Never20.621.519.121.8
    Light15.814.015.717.2
    Moderate20.319.521.918.9
    Regular24.526.524.522.9
    Vigorous regular18.818.518.819.1
Mean age, y (SE)42.6 (0.4)38.1 (0.7)***44.4 (0.8)44.2 (0.6)
Women, %53.051.450.357.4*

Note. BMI = body mass index.

a Full sample statistics excludes “other” racial group (n = 80).

b At risk was defined as a waist circumference greater than 102 cm for men and greater than 88 cm for women.

c Top 3 attributions besides race/ethnicity.

*P < .025; **P < .005; ***P < .001, compared with Whites.

About 25% of all respondents reported at least 1 experience of perceived racial/ethnic discrimination and about 40% of the sample reported at least 1 perceived experience of nonracial/nonethnic discrimination. The sample average masks great differences among the 3 racial/ethnic groups. For example, 42.9% of Blacks and 30.5% of Hispanics reported racial/ethnic discrimination, compared with only 7.8% of Whites. By contrast, 55.6% of Whites reported experiencing nonracial/nonethnic discrimination, compared with 27.3% of Hispanics and 34.8% of Blacks. Blacks reported a greater number of stressful major life events than did Hispanics and Whites (Table 1).

Perceived Discrimination and Excess Body Fat Accumulation

Table 2 presents the adjusted odds ratios (ORs) and the 95% confidence intervals (CIs) from the multivariate multinomial logistic regression analysis for the association between perceived discrimination and BMI. Model 1 adjusted for the sociodemographic and behavioral covariates. Model 2 additionally adjusted for major life events. Racial/ethnic discrimination was significantly associated with BMI, but only for Whites. Whites who reported experiencing any racial/ethnic discrimination were more likely to be obese than were Whites who experienced no racial/ethnic discrimination (model 2). Although experiences of nonracial/nonethnic discrimination were positively associated with obesity among Blacks and with overweight and obesity among Hispanics, none of these relationships were statistically significant. The association between experiences of stressful major life events and BMI was only statistically significant among Hispanics (Table 2, model 2). Each additional stressful major life event increased the risk of Hispanics being obese by 16%.

Table

TABLE 2 Predicted Probability of Overweight and Obesity, by Race/Ethnicity: Chicago Community Adult Health Study Sample, Chicago, IL, 2001–2003

TABLE 2 Predicted Probability of Overweight and Obesity, by Race/Ethnicity: Chicago Community Adult Health Study Sample, Chicago, IL, 2001–2003

Model 1a
Model 2b
Overweight,c OR (95% CI)Obese,d OR (95% CI)Overweight,c OR (95% CI)Obese,d OR (95% CI)
Hispanics
Any racial/ethnic discrimination1.22 (0.64, 2.31)1.35 (0.72, 2.55)1.14 (0.59, 2.20)1.14 (0.59, 2.18)
Any nonracial/nonethnic discrimination1.00 (0.53, 1.88)1.28 (0.69, 2.37)0.94 (0.49, 1.80)1.04 (0.55, 1.96)
Major life events1.04 (0.91, 1.19)1.16* (1.02, 1.31)
Non-Hispanic Whites
Any racial/ethnic discrimination1.28 (0.55, 3.00)2.43* (1.09, 5.40)1.25 (0.53, 2.98)2.19* (1.00, 4.80)
Any nonracial/nonethnic discrimination1.16 (0.73, 1.83)1.76* (1.02, 3.03)1.11 (0.70, 1.76)1.53 (0.90, 2.60)
Major life events1.03 (0.93, 1.15)1.12* (1.00, 1.24)
Non-Hispanic Blacks
Any racial/ethnic discrimination0.83 (0.46, 1.48)1.02 (0.61, 1.71)0.88 (0.49, 1.56)1.00 (0.60, 1.67)
Any nonracial/nonethnic discrimination0.96 (0.50, 1.83)1.46 (0.84, 2.55)1.03 (0.54, 1.97)1.41 (0.82, 2.43)
Major life events0.94 (0.86, 1.03)1.03 (0.95, 1.11)

Note. OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval. Reference category: underweight or normal (body mass index [BMI; weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared] less than 25.0 kg/m2).

a Model 1 adjusted for age, gender, income, education, smoking, drinking, and physical activity.

b Model 2 additionally adjusted for major life events.

c Overweight was defined as a BMI between 25.0 to 29.9 kg/m2.

d Obese was defined as a BMI of 30.0 kg/m2 or greater.

* P < .05.

Table 3 presents the adjusted ORs and the 95% CIs from the multivariate logistic regression for the high-risk waist circumference analysis. A similar pattern to the BMI analysis emerged for Whites in the high-risk waist circumference analysis (Table 3). However, among Whites, adjusting for BMI completely mediated the relationship between high-risk waist circumference and racial/ethnic discrimination, decreasing the odds of having a high-risk waist circumference from 2.87 (P < .01) to 1.90 (P > .05). The OR for reported experiences of nonracial/nonethnic discrimination, however, increased slightly from 2.02 to 2.11. There were no statistically significant relationships between discrimination and high-risk waist circumference for Blacks or Hispanics.

Table

TABLE 3 Predicted Probability of a High-Risk Waist Circumference, by Race/Ethnicity: Chicago Community Adult Health Study Sample, Chicago, IL, 2001–2003

TABLE 3 Predicted Probability of a High-Risk Waist Circumference, by Race/Ethnicity: Chicago Community Adult Health Study Sample, Chicago, IL, 2001–2003

Model 1,a OR (95% CI)Model 2,b OR (95% CI)Model 3,c OR (95% CI)
Hispanics
Any racial/ethnic discrimination1.30 (0.85, 1.99)1.17 (0.75, 1.81)1.12 (0.61, 2.05)
Any nonracial/nonethnic discrimination1.51 (0.95, 2.40)1.32 (0.82, 2.12)1.68 (0.77, 3.67)
Major life events1.11* (1.02, 1.21)1.01 (0.88, 1.16)
Non-Hispanic Whites
Any racial/ethnic discrimination3.23*** (1.67, 6.25)2.87** (1.47, 5.62)1.90 (0.75, 4.79)
Any nonracial/nonethnic discrimination2.39*** (1.49, 3.83)2.02** (1.25, 3.28)2.11* (1.09, 4.09)
Major life events1.14** (1.03, 1.25)1.12 (0.98, 1.27)
Non-Hispanic Blacks
Any racial/ethnic discrimination0.84 (0.55, 1.28)0.78 (0.51, 1.19)0.71 (0.39, 1.30)
Any nonracial/nonethnic discrimination1.20 (0.76, 1.89)1.09 (0.69, 1.70)0.88 (0.47, 1.63)
Major life events1.09* (1.02, 1.17)1.14* (1.02, 1.26)

Note. OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval. High-risk waist circumference was defined as greater than 102 cm for men and greater than 88 cm for women.

a Model 1 adjusted for age, gender, income, education, smoking, drinking, and physical activity.

b Model 2 additionally adjusted for major life events.

c Model 3 additionally adjusted for body mass index (weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared).

*P < .05; **P < .01; ***P < .001.

Similar to results from the BMI analyses, major life events slightly attenuated the relationship between perceived discrimination and high-risk waist circumference. However, unlike in the BMI analyses, experiences of stressful life events were positively associated with high-risk waist circumference among all 3 racial/ethnic groups (Table 3, model 2) after adjustment for all of the covariates except for BMI. However, after adjustment for BMI, the relationship was only significant (P < .05) for Blacks.

We conducted additional analyses to explore the finding that perceived discrimination was associated with excess body fat accumulation among Whites but neither among Blacks nor Hispanics. Because of the ethnic heterogeneity among Whites in the Chicago sample, in our posthoc analyses, we focused on several subpopulations of Whites. Previous research suggests that “discriminated-against Whites,” ethnic Whites that had historically been at elevated risk of discrimination in the United States, currently report higher levels of discrimination than do other Whites.69 We defined ethnicity to include ancestral and geographical origins, cultural traditions, or languages.70,71 On the basis of similar typologies suggested by other studies,54,72,73 we created a dichotomized variable, contrasting Whites who reported their ethnicity as Polish, Irish, Italian, or Jewish (ethnic Whites) to all other Whites (other Whites). To determine if the relationship between perceived experiences of discrimination and excess body fat differed among ethnic Whites (unweighted n = 327) and all other Whites (unweighted n = 656), we conducted separate logistic regression analyses stratified by these 2 groups of Whites.

Although similar proportions of both groups of Whites perceived ethnic and non-ethnic discrimination (data not shown), subsequent analyses did show that the relationship between perceived discrimination and high-risk waist circumference differed between the 2 groups (Table 4). Ethnic Whites who reported perceived ethnic discrimination were more than 6 times as likely to have a high-risk waist circumference than were ethnic Whites who reported no ethnic discrimination. Similarly, ethnic Whites who reported nonethnic discrimination were more than 2 times as likely to have a high-risk waist circumference than were ethnic Whites who did not experience nonethnic discrimination. Although the odds of having a high-risk waist circumference were elevated (OR > 1.0) among the group of other Whites who reported perceived discrimination compared with those who reported no discrimination, the discrimination measures were not statistically significant after we controlled for all of the covariates.

Table

TABLE 4 Predicted Probability of a High-Risk Waist Circumference Among Whites: Chicago Community Adult Health Study Sample, Chicago, IL, 2001–2003

TABLE 4 Predicted Probability of a High-Risk Waist Circumference Among Whites: Chicago Community Adult Health Study Sample, Chicago, IL, 2001–2003

Model 1,a OR (95% CI)Model 2,b OR (95% CI)Model 3,c OR (95% CI)
Ethnic Whitesd
Any racial/ethnic discrimination6.60** (2.07, 21.02)6.29** (1.91, 20.73)6.52** (1.59, 26.77)
Any nonracial/nonethnic discrimination3.02** (1.46, 6.25)2.77** (1.31, 5.85)2.61* (1.04, 6.56)
Major life events1.07 (0.89, 1.30)0.99 (0.80, 1.24)
Nonethnic Whites
Any racial/ethnic discrimination2.82* (1.21, 6.56)2.43* (1.04, 5.72)1.20 (0.39, 3.70)
Any nonracial/nonethnic discrimination2.19* (1.19, 4.00)1.77 (0.96, 3.28)1.98 (0.80, 4.89)
Major life events1.19** (1.05, 1.35)1.21* (1.01, 1.45)

Note. OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval. High-risk waist circumference was defined as greater than 102 cm for men and greater than 88 cm for women.

a Model 1 adjusted for age, gender, income, education, smoking, drinking, and physical activity.

b Model 2 additionally adjusted for major life events.

c Model 3 additionally adjusted for body mass index (weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared).

d Ethnic Whites comprises Irish, Italian, Jewish, and Polish Whites.

*P < .05; **P < .01; ***P < .001.

Interestingly, additional exploratory analyses not presented here indicated that the relationship between perceived discrimination and high-risk waist circumference observed in this study was only among White men and not among White women. However, because of the relatively large linearized standard errors stemming from the small sample size of men in the respective categories, we interpret this gender effect with caution.

Our study provides additional evidence to support the hypothesis that psychosocial stress is positively associated with excess body fat accumulation, at least under certain conditions.7,8 Consistent with other studies,9,13,16,41 our analyses show that perceived experiences of unfair treatment were associated with increased abdominal obesity after we controlled for other confounding characteristics. Contrary to our expectations, though, this pattern was only evident among Whites of Irish, Jewish, Polish, and Italian ancestry in Chicago. This finding is not completely unsupported, because these groups of Whites, in particular, have historically been discriminated against in the United States73 and other recent research suggests that they experience higher levels of discrimination than do other Whites and that these experiences adversely affect their health.69 However, because of the relatively small number of ethnic Whites reporting racial/ethnic discrimination, the parameter estimate for the racial/ethnic discrimination measure among ethnic Whites should be interpreted with some caution. Nonetheless, the strength of the association (P < .01) suggests that the findings are not because of chance.

Surprisingly, perceived discrimination was not statistically associated with excess body fat accumulation among Hispanics and Blacks. Although not significant, some of the patterns were in the opposite direction. It is possible, given the acceptance of greater body weight among Blacks and Hispanics compared with Whites, that cultural norms and experience rather than the stress of perceived discrimination is associated with body weight for these groups.

Alternatively, the observed patterns may be related to the socialization regarding experiences of discrimination, especially racial/ethnic discrimination. Blacks and Hispanics, who are socialized to expect discrimination, may have developed a wide variety of coping mechanisms that may be protective. Conversely, Whites, who are not socialized to expect discrimination, may have fewer coping resources to deal with such experiences or may use behavioral coping strategies that increase the risk of obesity such as consuming additional food or alcohol. That is, the variation in the relationship between perceived discrimination and high-risk waist circumference between ethnic Whites and the other groups may be because of the selection of varying coping mechanisms that may be differentially related to the outcome. For example, a study found that Blacks who recognized and verbalized interpersonal experiences of discrimination were less likely to be hypertensive compared with Blacks who did not report any perceived experiences of discrimination.16,24 Research needs to delineate the full range of coping responses to perceptions of discrimination and identify how these may vary by race and ethnicity. It is worth noting that we also explored whether discrimination was related to obesity in ethnic subgroups of Blacks and Hispanics; however, none of these relationships was significant.

The single previous study that examined perceived discrimination and body fat found that discrimination and waist-to-hip ratio were inversely associated with each other in a sample of Black women.16 Although in our study perceived discrimination also appeared to be inversely (OR < 1.0) associated with excess body fat accumulation for Blacks, none of the parameter estimates was statistically significant. The null findings among all Blacks and Black women in our study (data not shown) may be attributed to several reasons. First, the observed differences may be an artifact of the variation in the perceived discrimination instruments used by Vines et al.16 and in our study. The scale used in our study, for example, assessed day-to-day experiences of perceived discrimination, whereas the scale used in the other study16 was designed to capture both day-to-day and acute experiences of perceived discrimination of Black women.

Second, whereas the discrimination measure in our study captured the occurrence and frequency of personal experiences of perceived unfair treatment, the other study16 utilized a multidimensional measure that assessed the respondents' personal experiences and their perceptions of racism experienced by Blacks. Last, the age range in the 2 samples was quite different. The other study16 was of Black women aged 35 to 49 years, whereas the age of Black women in our study was 18 to 92 years. However, because none of the relationships between discrimination and any of the excess body fat measures in our study was statistically significant among Blacks and Hispanics, we interpret the our study's results for these 2 groups cautiously.

We were also interested in the relationship between stressful major life events and excess body fat accumulation. Several observations are worth noting. First, stressful major life events were not consistently associated with the excess body fat measures used in this study. Stressful major life events were associated with BMI only among Hispanics and with high-risk waist circumference only among Blacks and the ethnic White group. Second, major life events, when significant, marginally attenuated the relationship between the discrimination measure and the excess body fat outcomes. Finally, although major life events were sometimes associated with a higher risk of excess body fat, these events were never associated with a lower risk of excess body fat.

Limitations

One of the major limitations of this study, as with all cross-sectional studies, is its inability to suggest temporal ordering among the relationships observed. Future research needs to elucidate the extent to which exposure to perceived discrimination predates the accumulation of excess body fat.

To address the concern of bias caused by obese individuals who reported discrimination because of their weight, we excluded these individuals (n = 33) in additional analyses. Because both approaches yielded similar results, we retained these individuals in the analyses presented here.

To safeguard against potentially spurious findings, we also explored the potential confounding with depression. Given that depression is associated with perceived discrimination and obesity, in analyses not shown, we added a scale of depressive symptoms (Center for Epidemiologic Studies depression scale74) to the model but it did not meaningfully alter our results. We also explored whether our results were caused by generational or immigrant status rather than ethnic status among Whites. These results (not shown) suggest that the ethnic classification used among Whites in this study was not a proxy for immigration or generational status.

Another potential limitation of our study concerns the accuracy of the self-reported discrimination events and the relationship of self-reported discrimination to health. This concern, albeit reasonable and likely widely shared, is not supported by the available evidence. One prospective study found that underlying psychological status at baseline is unrelated to future reports of perceived discrimination.26 The underreporting of interpersonal discrimination should also be of concern. Some evidence suggests that at least some socially disadvantaged groups cope with discrimination by minimizing or denying its occurrence (see Williams et al.22 and Paradies23 for review). If underreporting of perceived discrimination did occur in this study, the relationship between perceived discrimination and excess body fat accumulation would be biased downward. However, the results from this study do not rule out the possibility that reports of perceived discrimination were influenced by other psychosocial characteristics that may be related to excess body fat accumulation.

Conclusions

The results of this study provide some evidence to support the hypothesis that perceived interpersonal experiences of discrimination may be associated with excess body fat accumulation, at least for some population groups. The evidence suggests that perceived experiences of discrimination, a psychosocial stressor, are related to general body fatness and to visceral fat accumulation, particularly among Polish, Irish, Italian, and Jewish Whites in Chicago. Collectively, these observations warrant further investigation to answer the question, Under what conditions do particular psychosocial stressors matter for excess body fat accumulation and for which specific groups?22 Additionally, future research should seek to identify the psychological or behavioral factors that may mediate or moderate the relationship between perceived discrimination and excess body fat accumulation.22 The results of this and other studies suggest that a multidisciplinary research agenda that directly examines the physiological pathways of psychosocial stressors, including perceived interpersonal experiences of discrimination, and obesity is warranted.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development of the National Institutes of Health (grants P50HD38986 and R01HD050467), with additional support from the MacArthur Foundation (via Harvard University and the University of California, San Francisco) and the US Department of Justice (via Harvard University). The authors thank the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Health & Society Scholars program for its financial support during the writing of this article.

Part of this article was originally conceived as a part of H. E. R. Hunte's dissertation in 2006. He would especially like to thank the members of his dissertation committee: Paula M. Lantz, Jersey Liang, James S. House, and David R. Williams. Previous versions of this article were presented at the Purdue University's Department of Health and Kinesiology Colloquium Series in 2007 and the Institute on Aging and the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Health and Society Seminar Series in 2007, both at the University of Wisconsin, Madison.

The authors also thank the anonymous reviewers during the peer review process for their helpful comments.

Note. The contents of the manuscript are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the aforementioned institutions or individuals.

Human Participant Protection

The analysis conducted for this article was exempt from institutional review by the institutional review board at the University of Wisconsin, Madison. The data collection process was approved by the institutional review board for human participant protection at the University of Michigan.

References

1. Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Curtin LR, McDowell MA, Tabak CJ, Flegal KM. Prevalence of overweight and obesity in the United States, 1999–2004. JAMA. 2006;295:15491555. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
2. Flegal KM, Carroll MD, Ogden CL, Johnson CL. Prevalence and trends in obesity among US adults, 1999–2000. JAMA. 2002;288:17231727. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
3. [No authors listed.] Overweight, obesity, and health risk. National Task Force on the Prevention and Treatment of Obesity. Arch Intern Med. 2000;160:898904. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
4. The Practical Guide: Identification, Evaluation, and Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adults. Bethesda, MD: National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, National Institutes of Health; 2000. Google Scholar
5. Hedley AA, Ogden CL, Johnson CL, Carroll MD, Curtin LR, Flegal KM. Prevalence of overweight and obesity among US children, adolescents, and adults, 1999–2002. JAMA. 2004;291:28472850. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
6. Nishitani N, Sakakibara H. Relationship of obesity to job stress and eating behavior in male Japanese workers. Int J Obes (Lond). 2006;30:528533. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
7. Björntorp P. Visceral fat accumulation: the missing link between psychosocial factors and cardiovascular disease? J Intern Med. 1991;230:195201. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
8. Björntorp P. Do stress reactions cause abdominal obesity and comorbidities? Obes Rev. 2001;2:7386. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
9. Chambers EC, Tull ES, Fraser HS, Mutunhu NR, Sobers N, Niles E. The relationship of internalized racism to body fat distribution and insulin resistance among African adolescent youth. J Natl Med Assoc. 2004;96:15941598. MedlineGoogle Scholar
10. Dallman MF, Pecoraro N, Akana SF, et al.. Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of “comfort food.” Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:1169611701. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
11. Epel E, Lapidus R, McEwen B, Brownell K. Stress may add bite to appetite in women: a laboratory study of stress-induced cortisol and eating behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001;26:3749. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
12. Fabricatore AN, Wadden TA. Psychological aspects of obesity. Clin Dermatol. 2004;22:332337. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
13. Tull SE, Wickramasuriya T, Taylor J, et al.. Relationship of internalized racism to abdominal obesity and blood pressure in Afro-Caribbean women. J Natl Med Assoc. 1999;91:447452. MedlineGoogle Scholar
14. Marniemi J, Kronholm E, Aunola S, et al.. Visceral fat and psychosocial stress in identical twins discordant for obesity. J Intern Med. 2002;251:3543. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
15. Rohrer JE, Rohland BM. Psychosocial risk factors for obesity among women in a family planning clinic. BMC Fam Pract. 2004;5:20. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
16. Vines AI, Baird DD, Stevens J, Hertz-Picciotto I, Light KC, McNeilly M. Associations of abdominal fat with perceived racism and passive emotional responses to racism in African American women. Am J Public Health. 2007;97:526530. LinkGoogle Scholar
17. McEwen BS, Seeman T. Protective and damaging effects of mediators of stress. Elaborating and testing the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896:3047. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
18. Kelly S, Hertzman C, Daniels M. Searching for the biological pathways between stress and health. Annu Rev Public Health. 1997;18:437462. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
19. Peters A, Pellerin L, Dallman MF, et al.. Causes of obesity: looking beyond the hypothalamus. Prog Neurobiol. 2007;81:6188. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
20. McEwen BS, Stellar E. Stress and the individual. Mechanisms leading to disease. Arch Intern Med. 1993;153:20932101. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
21. Mays VM, Cochran SD, Barnes NW. Race, race-based discrimination, and health outcomes among African Americans. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:201225. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
22. Williams DR, Neighbors HW, Jackson JS. Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: findings from community studies. Am J Public Health. 2003;93:200208. LinkGoogle Scholar
23. Paradies Y. A systematic review of empirical research on self-reported racism and health. Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35:888901. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
24. Krieger N, Sidney S. Racial discrimination and blood pressure: the CARDIA Study of young Black and White adults. Am J Public Health. 1996;86:13701378. LinkGoogle Scholar
25. Brondolo E, Rieppi R, Kelly KP, Gerin W. Perceived racism and blood pressure: a review of the literature and conceptual and methodological critique. Ann Behav Med. 2003;25:5565. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
26. Brown TN, Williams DR, Jackson JS, et al.. “Being Black and feeling blue”: the mental health consequences of racial discrimination. Race Soc. 1999;2:117131. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
27. Noh S, Beiser M, Kaspar V, Hou F, Rummens J. Perceived racial discrimination, depression, and coping: a study of Southeast Asian refugees in Canada. J Health Soc Behav. 1999;40:193207. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
28. Jackson JS, Brown TN, Williams DR, Torres M, Sellers SL, Brown K. Racism and the physical and mental health status of African Americans: a thirteen year national panel study. Ethn Dis. 1996;6:132147. MedlineGoogle Scholar
29. Bennett GG, Wolin KY, Robinson EL, Fowler S, Edwards CL. Perceived racial/ethnic harassment and tobacco use among African American young adults. Am J Public Health. 2005;95:238240. LinkGoogle Scholar
30. Guthrie BJ, Young AM, Williams DR, Boyd CJ, Kintner EK. African American girls' smoking habits and day-to-day experiences with racial discrimination. Nurs Res. 2002;51:183190. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
31. Landrine H, Klonoff EA. Racial discrimination and cigarette smoking among Blacks: findings from two studies. Ethn Dis. 2000;10:195202. MedlineGoogle Scholar
32. Yen IH, Ragland DR, Greiner BA, Fisher JM. Racial discrimination and alcohol-related behavior in urban transit operators: findings from the San Francisco Muni Health and Safety Study. Public Health Rep. 1999;114:448458. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
33. Yen IH, Ragland DR, Greiner BA, Fisher JM. Workplace discrimination and alcohol consumption: findings from the San Francisco Muni Health and Safety Study. Ethn Dis. 1999;9:7080. MedlineGoogle Scholar
34. Collins JW, David RJ, Handler A, Wall S, Andes S. Very low birthweight in African American infants: the role of maternal exposure to interpersonal racial discrimination. Am J Public Health. 2004;94:21322138. LinkGoogle Scholar
35. Mustillo S, Krieger N, Gunderson EP, Sidney S, McCreath H, Kiefe CI. Self-reported experiences of racial discrimination and Black-White differences in preterm and low-birthweight deliveries: the CARDIA Study. Am J Public Health. 2004;94:21252131. LinkGoogle Scholar
36. Wyatt SB, Williams DR, Calvin R, Henderson FC, Walker ER, Winters K. Racism and cardiovascular disease in African Americans. Am J Med Sci. 2003;325:315331. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
37. Troxel WM, Matthews KA, Bromberger JT, Sutton-Tyrrell K. Chronic stress burden, discrimination, and subclinical carotid artery disease in African American and Caucasian women. Health Psychol. 2003;22:300309. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
38. Lewis TT, Everson-Rose SA, Powell LH, et al.. Chronic exposure to everyday discrimination and coronary artery calcification in African-American women: the SWAN Heart Study. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:362368. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
39. Jones CP. Levels of racism: a theoretic framework and a gardener's tale. Am J Public Health. 2000;90:12121215. LinkGoogle Scholar
40. Williams DR, Williams-Morris R. Racism and mental health: the African American experience. Ethn Health. 2000;5:243268. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
41. Butler C, Tull ES, Chambers EC, Taylor J. Internalized racism, body fat distribution, and abnormal fasting glucose among African-Caribbean women in Dominica, West Indies. J Natl Med Assoc. 2002;94:143148. MedlineGoogle Scholar
42. Stuber J, Galea S, Ahern J, Blaney S, Fuller C. The association between multiple domains of discrimination and self-assessed health: a multilevel analysis of Latinos and Blacks in four low-income New York City neighborhoods. Health Serv Res. 2003;38(6 pt 2):17351759. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
43. Gee GC, Ryan A, Laflamme DJ, Holt J. Self-reported discrimination and mental health status among African descendants, Mexican Americans, and other Latinos in the New Hampshire REACH 2010 Initiative: the added dimension of immigration. Am J Public Health. 2006;96:18211828. LinkGoogle Scholar
44. Ryan AM, Gee GC, Laflamme DF. The association between self-reported discrimination, physical health and blood pressure: findings from African Americans, Black immigrants, and Latino immigrants in New Hampshire. J Health Care Poor Underserved. 2006;17(2 suppl):116132. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
45. Kessler RC, Mickelson KD, Williams DR. The prevalence, distribution, and mental health correlates of perceived discrimination in the United States. J Health Soc Behav. 1999;40:208230. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
46. Barnes LL, Mendes De Leon CF, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Bennett DA, Evans DA. Racial differences in perceived discrimination in a community population of older Blacks and Whites. J Aging Health. 2004;16:315337. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
47. Schulz A, Israel B, Williams D, Parker E, Becker A, James S. Social inequalities, stressors and self reported health status among African American and White women in the Detroit metropolitan area. Soc Sci Med. 2000;51:16391653. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
48. Gee GC. A multilevel analysis of the relationship between institutional and individual racial discrimination and health status. Am J Public Health. 2002;92:615623. LinkGoogle Scholar
49. Gee GC, Spencer M, Chen J, Yip T, Takeuchi DT. The association between self-reported racial discrimination and 12-month DSM-IV mental disorders among Asian Americans nationwide. Soc Sci Med. 2007;64:19841996. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
50. Casey S, O'Connell M. Pain and prejudice: assessing the experience of racism in Ireland. In: , MacLachlan M, O'Connell M, eds. Cultivating Pluralism: Psychological, Social and Cultural Perspectives on a Changing Ireland. Dublin, Ireland: Oak Tree Press; 2000:1948. Google Scholar
51. Dion KL, Dion KK, Pak AW. Personality-based hardiness as a buffer for discrimination-related stress in members of Toronto's Chinese community. Can J Behav Sci. 1992;24:517536. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
52. Karlsen S, Nazroo JY. Relation between racial discrimination, social class, and health among ethnic minority groups. Am J Public Health. 2002;92:624631. LinkGoogle Scholar
53. Harris R, Tobias M, Jeffreys M, Waldegrave K, Karlsen S, Nazroo J. Racism and health: the relationship between experience of racial discrimination and health in New Zealand. Soc Sci Med. 2006;63:14281441. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
54. Bhui K, Stansfeld S, McKenzie K, Karlsen S, Nazroo J, Weich S. Racial/ethnic discrimination and common mental disorders among workers: findings from the EMPIRIC Study of Ethnic Minority Groups in the United Kingdom. Am J Public Health. 2005;95:496501. LinkGoogle Scholar
55. De Vogli R, Ferrie JE, Chandola T, Kivimäki M, Marmot MG. Unfairness and health: evidence from the Whitehall II Study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61:513518. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
56. Meyer IH. Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychol Bull. 2003;129:674697. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
57. Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health: socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. J Health Psychol. 1997;2:335351. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
58. Morenoff JD, House JS, Hansen BB, Williams DR, Kaplan GA, Hunte HE. Understanding social disparities in hypertension prevalence, awareness, treatment, and control: the role of neighborhood context. Soc Sci Med. 2007;65:18531866. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
59. US Census Bureau. State and county quick facts. Available at: http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/states/17/1714000.html. Accessed June 7, 2008. Google Scholar
60. [No authors listed.] Plan and operation of the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1988–94. Series 1: programs and collection procedures. Vital Health Stat 1. 1994;July(32):1407. Google Scholar
61. Pietrobelli A, Boner AL, Tatò L. Adipose tissue and metabolic effects: new insight into measurements. Int J Obes (Lond). 2005;29(suppl 2):S97S100. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
62. Després JP, Moorjani S, Lupien PJ, Tremblay A, Nadeau A, Bouchard C. Regional distribution of body fat, plasma lipoproteins, and cardiovascular disease. Arteriosclerosis. 1990;10:497511. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
63. Kissebah AH, Vydelingum N, Murray R, et al.. Relation of body fat distribution to metabolic complications of obesity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1982;54:254260. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
64. Menke A, Muntner P, Wildman RP, Reynolds K, He J. Measures of adiposity and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2007;15:785795. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
65. Kuk JL, Katzmarzyk PT, Nichaman MZ, Church TS, Blair SN, Ross R. Visceral fat is an independent predictor of all-cause mortality in men. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2006;14:336341. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
66. Bosy-Westphal A, Geisler C, Onur S, et al.. Value of body fat mass vs anthropometric obesity indices in the assessment of metabolic risk factors. Int J Obes (Lond). 2006;30:475483. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
67. Chan DC, Watts GF, Barrett PH, Burke V. Waist circumference, waist-to-hip ratio and body mass index as predictors of adipose tissue compartments in men. QJM. 2003;96:441447. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
68. Schulz AJ, Gravlee CC, Williams DR, Israel BA, Mentz G, Rowe Z. Discrimination, symptoms of depression, and self-rated health among African American women in Detroit: results from a longitudinal analysis. Am J Public Health. 2006;96:12651270. LinkGoogle Scholar
69. Jackson JS, Williams DR, Torres M. Perceptions of discrimination, health and mental health: the social stress process. In: Socioeconomic Conditions, Stress and Mental Disorders: Toward a New Synthesis of Research and Public Policy 2002 [online collection]. Available at: http://www.mhsip.org/pdfs/jackson.pdf. Accessed June 7, 2008. Google Scholar
70. Bhopal R. Is research into ethnicity and health racist, unsound, or important science? BMJ. 1997;314:17511756. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
71. Jackson FL. Race and ethnicity as biological constructs. Ethn Dis. 1992;2:120125. MedlineGoogle Scholar
72. Aspinall PJ. Describing the “white” ethnic group and its composition in medical research. Soc Sci Med. 1998;47:17971808. Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
73. Waters MC. Ethnic Options: Choosing Identities in America. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1990. Google Scholar
74. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385401. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

Related

No related items

TOOLS

SHARE

ARTICLE CITATION

Haslyn E. R. Hunte, PhD, MPH, and David R. Williams, PhD, MPHAt the time of the study, Haslyn E. R. Hunte was with the Department of Population Health Sciences, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin, Madison. David R. Williams is with the Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, MA, and the Department of Society, Human Development and Health, Harvard University, Boston. “The Association Between Perceived Discrimination and Obesity in a Population-Based Multiracial and Multiethnic Adult Sample”, American Journal of Public Health 99, no. 7 (July 1, 2009): pp. 1285-1292.

https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2007.128090

PMID: 18923119